Optimal control and strong interactions imply flock cohesionNew work suggests that copying behavior in social groups may be governed by optimal control theory Nature has provided herding animals and flocking birds with abilities to react to predator attacks and to sense risky features in their environment— like trees or cliffs—that might impede their defense. But how do these abilities work? What’s going on inside individual animals and the group as a whole as they protect themselves with coordinated movements? New research by a University of Maryland alumnus and faculty member published in the current issue of the Proceedings of the Royal Society A explores how an avoidance signal from a single animal closest to the danger can quickly spread through the collective. The researchers developed mathematical models of optimal control that capture imitative behavior in a collective as if a “master clock” governs all group members. "Optimality, reduction and collective motion" (RSPA 20140606) was written by University of Maryland alumnus Eric Justh (EE Ph.D. 1998), Electronics Engineer at the Naval Research Laboratory; and Professor P. S. Krishnaprasad (ECE/ISR). “The nearly-in-unison movement of a herd of animals away from danger may be the result of rapid transfer of information in a collective,” says Krishnaprasad. “An individual close to danger senses it and signals avoidance to others through an action such as turning. Signals need to propagate, and in this case, the flock itself is the medium through which such information spreads in a wave of activity arising from an optimality principle.” When engineers and biologists make models of flocks, they envision them as composed of individual agents (self-steering particles) with free will to control actions such as turning or escape based on perceptual feedback—what they sense is going on in the environment. A collaborator in Italy—Dr. Andrea Cavagna—shared three-dimensional position data from observations of large flocks of European starlings (numbering in the hundreds) displaying striking flight behavior. His team’s analysis of the data investigates information transfer in a flock using Hamilton’s equations arising from the application of statistical physics principles. The Maryland team formulated an optimal control problem for flocking and derived Hamilton’s equations based on principles of optimality associated with Pontryagin and others. “We have already used optimal control principles in solving inverse problems such as reconstructing smooth trajectories from sampled data of natural flocks,” says Krishnaprasad. “We expect insights from optimal control could yield testable predictions on information transfer in some flocking events. This is work in progress.” These insights from the study of nature have practical implications. For example, they can be used to devise algorithms for collective behavior in robotics. Justh and Krishnaprasad’s findings may lead to the creation of new algorithms for networked robots to work together on tasks such as vigilance. Recent work in the Intelligent Servosystems Laboratory is pursuing these avenues of research. - - - - - - - - - - - The work of P. S. Krishnaprasad was supported in part by the Air Force Office of Scientific Research under AFOSR grant no. FA9550-10-1-0250, and the ARL/ARO MURI programme grant no. W911NF-13-1-0390. The work of Eric Justh was supported by the Office of Naval Research. - - - - - - - - - - - For more information Online paper Image Work of P. S. Krishnaprasad Videos Starling flocks near Oxford, England, from the website keepturningleft.co.uk More than 60,000 starlings in flight, filmed by Tuur Hofman in March 2011 near Utrecht, Netherlands
Related Articles: April 2, 2015 Prev Next |